000152041 001__ 152041
000152041 005__ 20250326144155.0
000152041 0247_ $$2doi$$a10.1371/journal.pone.0250105
000152041 0248_ $$2sideral$$a126622
000152041 037__ $$aART-2021-126622
000152041 041__ $$aeng
000152041 100__ $$0(orcid)0000-0002-1943-340X$$aMarín Manrique H.M$$uUniversidad de Zaragoza
000152041 245__ $$aBehavioural mimicry as an indicator of affiliation
000152041 260__ $$c2021
000152041 5060_ $$aAccess copy available to the general public$$fUnrestricted
000152041 5203_ $$aPrevious research has shown that behavioural mimicry fosters affiliation, and can be used to infer whether people belong to the same social unit. However, we still know very little about the generalizability of these findings and the individual factors involved. The present study intends to disentangle two important variables and assess their importance for affiliation: the matching in time of the behaviours versus their matching in form. In order to address this issue, we presented participants with short videos in which two actors displayed a set of small movements (e.g. crossing their legs, folding their arms, tapping their fingers) arranged to be either contingent in time or in form. A dark filter was used to eliminate ostensive group marks, such us phenotype or clothing. Participants attributed the highest degree of affiliation to the actors when their subsequent movements matched in form, but were delayed by 4-5 seconds, and the lowest degree when the timing of their movements matched, but they differed in form. To assess the generalizability of our findings, we took our study outside the usual Western context and tested a matching sample of participants from a traditional small-scale society in Kenya. In all, our results suggest that movements are used to judge the degree of affiliation between two individuals in both large- and small-scale societies. While moving in different ways at the same time seems to increase the perceived distance between two individuals, movements which match in form seem to invoke closeness.
000152041 540__ $$9info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess$$aby$$uhttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/es/
000152041 590__ $$a3.752$$b2021
000152041 591__ $$aMULTIDISCIPLINARY SCIENCES$$b29 / 74 = 0.392$$c2021$$dQ2$$eT2
000152041 592__ $$a0.852$$b2021
000152041 593__ $$aMultidisciplinary$$c2021$$dQ1
000152041 655_4 $$ainfo:eu-repo/semantics/article$$vinfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
000152041 700__ $$aMarín A.
000152041 700__ $$aNieto-Alemán P.A.
000152041 700__ $$aRead D.W.
000152041 700__ $$aHernández-Jaramillo J.
000152041 700__ $$aGarcía-Palacios A.
000152041 700__ $$aZeidler H.
000152041 7102_ $$14009$$2735$$aUniversidad de Zaragoza$$bDpto. Psicología y Sociología$$cÁrea Psicolog.Evolut.Educac
000152041 773__ $$g16, 5 (2021), [11 pp]$$pPLoS One$$tPLoS ONE$$x1932-6203
000152041 8564_ $$s705401$$uhttps://zaguan.unizar.es/record/152041/files/texto_completo.pdf$$yVersión publicada
000152041 8564_ $$s2265343$$uhttps://zaguan.unizar.es/record/152041/files/texto_completo.jpg?subformat=icon$$xicon$$yVersión publicada
000152041 909CO $$ooai:zaguan.unizar.es:152041$$particulos$$pdriver
000152041 951__ $$a2025-03-26-13:53:51
000152041 980__ $$aARTICLE